This article is from the source 'guardian' and was first published or seen on . It last changed over 40 days ago and won't be checked again for changes.

You can find the current article at its original source at https://www.theguardian.com/science/2017/jun/30/texting-on-a-mobile-phone-makes-you-walk-sillily-study-finds

The article has changed 5 times. There is an RSS feed of changes available.

Version 3 Version 4
Texting on a mobile phone makes you walk sillily, study finds Texting on a mobile phone makes you walk sillily, study finds
(11 days later)
Participants adopted ‘cautious stepping strategy’ while using device for different tasks, say university researchers
Nicola Davis
Fri 30 Jun 2017 19.00 BST
Last modified on Mon 27 Nov 2017 20.55 GMT
Share on Facebook
Share on Twitter
Share via Email
View more sharing options
Share on LinkedIn
Share on Pinterest
Share on Google+
Share on WhatsApp
Share on Messenger
Close
Texting on the hoof leads people to change the way they walk, new research has revealed.Texting on the hoof leads people to change the way they walk, new research has revealed.
While researchers have previously looked at the impact of phone use while on a level surface, they have now explored how pedestrians cope while using their phone and negotiating that common trip-hazard: a step.While researchers have previously looked at the impact of phone use while on a level surface, they have now explored how pedestrians cope while using their phone and negotiating that common trip-hazard: a step.
Injuries from falling over or bumping into things while using a mobile phone are on the rise: in the US at least 1,500 pedestrians were reported to have visited hospital in 2010 alone as a result of such accidents.Injuries from falling over or bumping into things while using a mobile phone are on the rise: in the US at least 1,500 pedestrians were reported to have visited hospital in 2010 alone as a result of such accidents.
“Part of it evolved from walking down the road in the middle of the day,” said co-author of the research Matthew Timmis of Anglia Ruskin University in Chelmsford, Essex. “The person in front of me was walking very slowly and weaving, and I thought: it’s quite early in the day: is this person intoxicated already?”“Part of it evolved from walking down the road in the middle of the day,” said co-author of the research Matthew Timmis of Anglia Ruskin University in Chelmsford, Essex. “The person in front of me was walking very slowly and weaving, and I thought: it’s quite early in the day: is this person intoxicated already?”
When he walked past, Timmis found the person was using their phone.When he walked past, Timmis found the person was using their phone.
To explore the impact of phone engagement on walking, the team kitted out 21 participants with a head-mounted eye tracker and motion-analysis sensors, and asked them to walk 5.6 metres, negotiating an obstacle made of fibreboard and a step made of a box on the way.To explore the impact of phone engagement on walking, the team kitted out 21 participants with a head-mounted eye tracker and motion-analysis sensors, and asked them to walk 5.6 metres, negotiating an obstacle made of fibreboard and a step made of a box on the way.
Participants were asked to go around the course three times without a phone, and repeat while writing a text message, reading a text message or making a call three times, making a total of 12 turns each.Participants were asked to go around the course three times without a phone, and repeat while writing a text message, reading a text message or making a call three times, making a total of 12 turns each.
The results, published in the journal Plos One by researchers at Anglia Ruskin and the University of Essex, revealed that fiddling with a phone increased how long people took to complete the task, decreased how often people looked at the step and changed how they walked.The results, published in the journal Plos One by researchers at Anglia Ruskin and the University of Essex, revealed that fiddling with a phone increased how long people took to complete the task, decreased how often people looked at the step and changed how they walked.
Participants adopted “an increasingly cautious stepping strategy” when using the phone and deviated from a straight path more often. While the effects were seen to various degrees for all three phone-use tasks, it was most pronounced when participants were asked to write a text message while walking.Participants adopted “an increasingly cautious stepping strategy” when using the phone and deviated from a straight path more often. While the effects were seen to various degrees for all three phone-use tasks, it was most pronounced when participants were asked to write a text message while walking.
They took 118% longer to complete the course when tapping out a text than when no phone was used. Participants were also found to fixate on the step 60% more and 91% longer, and fixate on the travel path 51% more, when phone-free.They took 118% longer to complete the course when tapping out a text than when no phone was used. Participants were also found to fixate on the step 60% more and 91% longer, and fixate on the travel path 51% more, when phone-free.
Those simply talking with the phone to their ear were also found to show some odd behaviour. “We expect when we walk in an environment you are going to look on the ground, you are going to look at the obstacle or the hazard that is coming, and look ahead to check you are wandering in the right direction,” said Timmis. “But when these guys were talking on the phone, they began to look at a bunch of other stuff that didn’t matter.”Those simply talking with the phone to their ear were also found to show some odd behaviour. “We expect when we walk in an environment you are going to look on the ground, you are going to look at the obstacle or the hazard that is coming, and look ahead to check you are wandering in the right direction,” said Timmis. “But when these guys were talking on the phone, they began to look at a bunch of other stuff that didn’t matter.”
While no one stumbled on the step in the tasks, the team found that the gait of participants was different when texting, talking or reading on the phone compared with when no phone was used – a result, says Timmis, of phone users spending less time looking at their environment, meaning they were relying on less-detailed peripheral vision.While no one stumbled on the step in the tasks, the team found that the gait of participants was different when texting, talking or reading on the phone compared with when no phone was used – a result, says Timmis, of phone users spending less time looking at their environment, meaning they were relying on less-detailed peripheral vision.
Approaching the step, both the participants’ leading and trailing foot was closer to the front edge of the step, while the stride length for both feet while walking was 38% shorter when texting than for when no phone was used. On tackling the step, those texting were also found to lift their lead foot 18% higher, and step more slowly.Approaching the step, both the participants’ leading and trailing foot was closer to the front edge of the step, while the stride length for both feet while walking was 38% shorter when texting than for when no phone was used. On tackling the step, those texting were also found to lift their lead foot 18% higher, and step more slowly.
“That is very much the hallmark of what we term a ‘safety strategy’,” said Timmis. The strategy, he added, reduced the cognitive burden on the working memory, freeing up resources for engaging with a phone – with more resources needing for texting than making a call.“That is very much the hallmark of what we term a ‘safety strategy’,” said Timmis. The strategy, he added, reduced the cognitive burden on the working memory, freeing up resources for engaging with a phone – with more resources needing for texting than making a call.
Timmis said that while the research showed phone-users had adopted a cautious walk, he warned that fiddling with a device could still result in accidents.Timmis said that while the research showed phone-users had adopted a cautious walk, he warned that fiddling with a device could still result in accidents.
“The big risk here is suddenly-appearing hazards [like] a pedestrian suddenly walking in front of you,” he said. “You are not going to be able to respond to that as efficiently, which increases the risk of injury.”“The big risk here is suddenly-appearing hazards [like] a pedestrian suddenly walking in front of you,” he said. “You are not going to be able to respond to that as efficiently, which increases the risk of injury.”
ScienceScience
HealthHealth
Share on FacebookShare on Facebook
Share on TwitterShare on Twitter
Share via EmailShare via Email
Share on LinkedInShare on LinkedIn
Share on PinterestShare on Pinterest
Share on Google+Share on Google+
Share on WhatsAppShare on WhatsApp
Share on MessengerShare on Messenger
Reuse this contentReuse this content